Ornithology
 

Digestive System: Food & Feeding Habits 


Hit 'Reload' or 'Refresh' to View Again!

Because of their high metabolic rates, birds must consume more food in proportion to their size than most animals. For example, a warbler might eat 80 percent of its body weight in a day. As a group, birds consume just about any type of food you can imagine, including amphibians, ants, buds, carrion, crustaceans, fish, fruit, grass, insects, larvae, leaves, molluscs, nectar, other birds, pollen, reptiles, rodents, roots, sap, seeds, suet, snails, wax, & worms. Birds acquire these food items in a wide variety of ways.


Major foraging guilds of birds. Vertivores prey primarily on vertebrates and invertivores on invertebrates, granivores are primarily seed eaters,
frugivores feed primarily on fruit, scavengers feed on carrion, nectarivores primarily on nectar, herbivores on plants, and aquatic predators on a wide variety of aquatic prey
(Figure from Pigot et al. 2020).


(a) Seven major foraging niches used by terrestrial invertivores, and (b) six major foraging niches used by aquatic predators (Figure from Pigot et al. 2020).


 

For birds that fly, mass affects flying ability so, in general, they need to keep their weight at some optimal level. That means that, except perhaps prior to migration, there is a limit to the amount of fat a bird can store. 'Efficient' means that birds must locate, ingest, & digest food as quickly and efficiently as possible.

Retention time (in hours) for fluid & particulate digesta markers in the gastrointestinal tracts 
of representative reptiles, birds, & mammals (Based on: Stevens and Hume 1998).
Species Body mass
Fluid retention time (hr)
Particle retention time (hr)  
Iguana
-
<48
207
Broad-tailed Hummingbird
3.3 gm
1.2
-
Rock Ptarmigan
460 gm
9.9
1.9
Sooty Albatross
2.5 kg
6.3
15
Rockhopper Penguin
2.5 kg
3.8
17
Emu
38 kg
3.9
4.7
rabbit
2.1kg
39
27
pig
176 kg
39
48
1McWhorter and Martinez del Rio (2000)

In general, typical mean retention times are 30 - 50 minutes for avian nectarivores, 40 - 100 minutes
for granivores, and 15 - 60 minutes for frugivores (Karasov 1990, Klasing 1998).


Retention times of fluids (solid lines) and particulate materials (dashed lines) markers in the gastrointestinal system of birds (triangles)
is shorter than that of mammals (squares). Even with shorter retention times, the digestive efficiency (energy extracted relative to food intake)
of birds is similar to that of mammals (Figure from McWhorter et al. 2009).

Digestive systems of a goose and a turkey and time needed for materials to move through the digestive systems

Drawings of the digestive tracts of (A) a Greylag Goose and ( B) a Wild Turkey and retention times of a solute, 2-mm particles, and 8-mm particles in the goose and turkey digestive systems
(Figure from Frei et al. 2017).


Here's one example of an avian digestive system:



Digestive system of a Domestic Chicken. The major components of the avian digestive system are the alimentary canal plus several accessory structures.
The 'canal' includes the oral cavity, pharynx, esophagus (which includes a crop in some birds), stomach (proventriculus & gizzard), small intestine, & large intestine. The large intestine then empties into the cloaca. Important accessory structures include the beak, salivary glands, liver, & pancreas.


A bird's bill consists of a bony framework covered by a tough layer of keratin (called the rhamphotheca). The keratin layer is continuously replaced throughout the life of a bird & is just as continuously worn down by eating and manipulating objects. Birds use their bills in a wide variety of ways, including preening, vocal modulation, thermoregulation, nest construction, tool use (for a few species), and, of course, food acquisition. Most birds have fast gapes and relatively weak bites, with bills serving as versatile, tweezer-like clamps (Navalon et al. 2018). Fewer birds have slower gapes and high biting force (Navalon et al. 2018). Examples of the latter include parrots and other seed-eating birds, plus some birds that often kill prey with their bills (e.g., falcons and shrikes).

Drawing of the skull of a Steller's Sea Eagle

Ligaments & muscles on the skull of a Steller's Sea Eagle.
Muscles that close the jaw include the adductor mandibulae externus, adductor mandibulae posterior,  & pterygoideus.
The depressor mandibulae opens the jaw (Source: Ladyguin 2000)

Drawings of the skull of a Cooper's Hawk showing main jaw muscles
Muscles involved in jaw closure for a Cooper's Hawk. A. Lateral view showing adductor mandibulae group, including
adductor mandibulae externus pars profunda (AMEP), adductor mandibulae ossis quadrati (AMOQ), adductor mandibulae
externus pars rostralis (AMER), adductor mandibulae pars ventralis (AMEV). B. Antero-lateral view showing the
pseudotemporalis group, including adductor mandibulae caudalis (AMC), pseudotemporalis profundus (PSP),
pseuodotemporalis superficialis (PSS), pterygoideus pars dorsalis (PTD), and pterygoideus pars ventralis (PTV)
(Sustaita 2008).


Jaw musculature of a White-bellied Parrot
(Figure modified from Sereno et al. 2010).


High bite forces are generated when the moment arm of the muscles’ input force is relatively high
(Figure from Van Wassenbergh and Baeckens 2019).


Cranial kinesis (and rhynchokinesis)

Variation in bill morphology

Drawing showing variation in the morphology of bird bills
Source: http://wwwvet.murdoch.edu.au/Anatomy/avian/billsl.GIF
Flamingos use a series of projections, or lamellae, to filter tiny food items from debris in the water.

Swifts are aerial insectivores & use their wide gape to help capture flying insects.

Eagles (and hawks) are diurnal raptors & use their hook-like bills to tear apart large prey.

Shovelers use their spatula-shaped bills to filter food from mud & water.

Crossbills use their 'crossed-bill' to extract seeds from pine cones.

Herons use their bills to capture small fish and amphibians.

Avocets sweep their long
up-curved bills from side-to-side through the water to capture small  invertebrates
(or use it like a forceps to pick up prey).

Woodpeckers use their chisel-like bills to chop away wood & expose insects and insect larvae.

Wrens use their thin, probing bill to capture small insects. 

Curlews use their long bill to probe mudflats for small invertebrates.

Hawfinches are seed-eaters & use their bills to crack open large, hard seeds.

Macaws use their strong hook-like bills to feed on nuts. 

Mallards & other waterfowl use their bills to filter small invertebrates from mud and water.

Skimmers use their elongated lower mandible to skim the surface of the water & capture small fish and invertebrates.

 


 
A kingfisher using its bill to capture prey!

 

Great Egret (slow motion)  

 
African Fish Eagle

 

 


 
Golden Eagle vs. a goat
(and a Wild Turkey)


 
Red-tailed Hawk eating a red squirrel

Foraging Wilson's Storm-Petrels


Photo of a sea-anchor soarting storm-petrel

Sea-anchor 'soaring' by storm-petrels -- As described by Pennycuick (2008), storm-petrels have a unique method for
picking up food from the water's surface. They glide into the wind with their body clear of the surface, but with their webbed feet
in the water, helping to 'anchor' them. Aerodynamic (or air) drag on on wings (friction drag) and body (parasitic drag) push
them backward through the water so that the feet tend to slowly move backward through the water, and that creates forward-directed
hydrodynamic (or water) drag. When this water drag balances the aerodynamic (air) drag, the storm-petrels remain in place, suspended above
the water's surface at a height that allows them to pick small food items from the surface (Photo source: Wikipedia).

 



Pale-billed Woodpecker
(Mayflower Bocawina National Park, Belize


A Pileated Woodpecker striking a wood substrate with its bill (times are in milliseconds). Note how the force of the impact causes
the woodpecker’s crest to move forward (from 63.1 to 68.1 ms), after which the crest feathers return to their original position (87.5 ms) (From Ross 2014).

Woodpecker shock-absorbing system -- Woodpeckers are known to drum hard woody surfaces of trees at a rate of 18 to 22 times per second with a deceleration of 1200 g (humans can lose consciousness at g-forces as low as 4 to 6 g). Woodpeckers have four structures that help absorb mechanical shock and prevent brain damage: (1) a hard, but elastic, beak, (2) a hyoid apparatus that rigidly supports the tongue, (3) an area of spongy bone located at the contrecoup position from the beak, and (4) skull bone with cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). A woodpecker's hyoid extends posteriorly from the floor of the oral cavity, goes behind the neck, divides into two bands, goes around the back of the skull, and inserts at the front of the skull. This allows woodpeckers to extend their tongues well beyond the tip of the beak when foraging, but this arrangement also helps distribute mechanical vibrations when drumming. The spongy bone is thought to evenly distribute mechanical vibrations before they propogate to the brain. Finally, woodpeckers have a very narrow space for CSF between the skull and brain, and little CSF helps reduce transmission of vibrations into the brain through the CSF (Source: Yoon and Park 2011).


Links:

Woodpecker's head inspires shock absorbers

Compare with humans


 

Images showing how the force of an impact travels along a woodpecker's bill and skull
The unequal length of the upper and lower parts of woodpecker beaks (the lower being longer) directs the force of impact downwards,
away from the brain, when it hits the tree (red coloration indicates the greatest force or stess; blue indicates the least force or stress). Time after impact proceeds from upper left (0.6 milliseconds after impact) to lower right (15 milliseconds after impact).

The hyoid of woodpeckers loops over top of the skull to completely surround their skulls. The hyoid helps direct the stress of impact below and around the skull (and brain) and also acts like a 'safety belt', helping to keep the skull and brain in place. It is the movement of the brain inside the skull during impact, more than the blow itself, that causes concussions. If the brain is held in place, injury risks are greatly reduced. As in the above figure, time after impact proceeds from upper left to lower right (Figures from Wang et al. 2011).

 

More recently, the results of a study by Van Wassenbergh et al. (2022) suggest that woodpecker brains do not require a shock-absorbing system
(https://www.cell.com/current-biology/pdf/S0960-9822(22)00855-7.pdf). However, it may be the case that, over a woodpecker's lifetime, the various mechanisms to minimize
shock described above may still prove beneficial in minimizing damage to woodpecker brains.



Red-necked Phalarope

 
The capillary ratchet mechanism

Surface tension transport of prey by feeding shorebirds: the capillary ratchet -- Recent work by Prakash et al. (2008) has revealed that phalaropes and other shorebirds take advantage of surface interactions between their beak and water droplets to propel bits of food from the tip of their long beaks to their mouth. They peck at the surface, picking up droplets of water with prey inside. Because their beaks point downward when feeding, gravity must be overcome to get those droplets from the tip of the bird's long beak to its mouth. This feeding strategy depends on surface tension. As the beak opens and closes, each movement propels the water droplet one step closer to the bird's mouth. Specifically, when the beak closes, the drop's leading edge moves toward the mouth, while the trailing edge stays put. In this stepwise ratcheting fashion, the drop travels along the beak at a speed of about 1 meter per second. The efficiency of the process, called the "capillary ratchet," depends on beak shape, and long, narrow beaks, like those of phalaropes, are best suited to this mode of feeding. - MIT News


Finches do not simply bite the seeds; instead; the lower mandible is moved toward the tip of the bill in a slicing motion. When most of the coat has been cracked or removed, the lower mandible is moved from side to side to remove the rest of the shell, thus releasing the kernel. Some large finches also have raised hard surfaces in the upper palate that function as anvils so large seeds can be held firmly while the lower mandible slices and cracks the sides of the seed. As tricky as nutcracking sounds, most birds accomplish it rapidly, shelling small seeds in a few seconds and large finches can crack open and devour a large seed or nut in less than 20 seconds.
Drawings showing how finches crack open seeds with their bill

Two photos showing how a Serin opens a seed with its bill and tongue
Serin (Serinus serinus) with a seed positioned in its bill. Note how the tongue is used to hold the seed in position
(From: van der Meij 2005).

 


Photo of a gannet about to plunge into water
Photo source: http://www.homepages.mcb.net/wormwell/Gannet130604.htm

The plunge dive of the Cape Gannet -- Gannets have one of the most spectacular prey-capture behaviors of all marine predators,
plummeting from up to 30 m into the water, where they seize fish with their razor-sharp beaks. Ropert-Coudert et al. (2004) monitored
the biomechanics of plunge diving in 25 free-ranging Cape Gannets Morus capensis using a rapid-sampling acceleration and depth
recorder. Their data provide the first detailed description of this highly specialized foraging technique. They recorded no or a very low
deceleration when Gannets entered the water, which underlines the remarkable streamlining of this large bird. Birds use their momentum
to travel underwater at an average descent rate of 2.87 m/s (sd = ±1.53) before actively braking once they attain the desired depth (range:
0.3–9.7 m). Ropert-Coudert et al. (2004) showed that Gannets sometimes used either their wings or feet for underwater propulsion during
the course of 9.4% of the dives that had undulations in their depth profiles. After chasing prey, birds developed an upward momentum before
gliding passively back to the surface, making use of their buoyancy to complete the dive at the lowest possible energy cost.
(Check the Gannet videos at ARKive).


Northern Gannets

 

Drawing showing different methods birds have for capturing flying insects
Aerial insectivores -- Swifts depend on flying efficiently and maintaining high speed. Hawking insectivores, like flycatchers, depend on perches
located near prey, but they must be able to accelerate rapidly and be very maneuverable. Swallows combine these two strategies; they are fast,
maneuverable and able to accelerate when necessary (Warrick 1998).

 


Feet and talons

Although not part of the digestive system in an anatomical sense, some birds, like hawks and owls, use their feet and talons to capture prey. Typically, raptor prey are killed by the talons of the contracting foot being driven into their bodies; if required, the hooked bill is used to kill prey being held by the talons.

 
Bird of Prey, the multi award-winning feature-length documentary from the Cornell Lab of Ornithology, weaves a remarkable story of the world's rarest eagle species and the heroic individuals working tirelessly to save it. Since its release, Bird of Prey has been viewed by audiences around the world and throughout the Philippines where the film has become an invaluable tool for raising awareness and support for conservation of the critically endangered Philippine Eagle. To learn more about Philippine Eagles and how you can help support their conservation visit: https://www.philippineeaglefoundation.org.


Harpy Eagle

 

 

Photo showing the talons of six species of raptors
Talons of (left > right): Harpy Eagle, Golden Eagle, Bald Eagle, Great Horned Owl, Red-tailed Hawk, & Peregrine Falcon

 

The raptor digital tendon locking mechanism -- Digital tendons form a mechanical-locking mechanism in many birds that must maintain a degree of grip force, including perching, hanging, tree-climbing, and raptorial species. In raptors, powerful hindlimb muscles produce a strong grasp, and a tendon locking mechanism (TLM) helps sustain grip force. The components of the digital TLM include a 'textured' pad on the ventral surface of each flexor tendon that contains thousands of minute, rigid, well-defined projections called tubercles (see figure below). The neighboring portion of the surrounding tendon sheath contains a series of transversely running plicae (folds) that often have a proximal slant (i.e. towards the base of the toe). When the flexor tendons are pulled taut, and the digits flexed, the tubercle pad moves proximally over the stationary plicae on the sheath. When resistance to digital flexion is met, the locking elements intermesh and engage and the friction produced prevents slippage of the tendons. This permits digital flexion to be maintained with little or no muscular involvement (Einoder and Richardson 2006).


Action of the avian digital TLM: Top, digital extension, Bottom, digital flexion. This shows the movement of the talon, flexor and extensor tendons,
and the movement of the ventrally located tubercle pad relative to the stationary plicated sheath and phalangeal bone
(From: Einoder and Richardson 2006).


 
Accipitriformes (hawks & eagles) and Strigiformes (owls) differ in morphology, talon force, & hunting behavior -- Ward et al. (2002) examined the hindlimb morphology of six raptors (listed below) to determine if resource partitioning might be explained, at least in part, by morphological differences. One difference is that the digit pattern in Strigiformes is zygodactylous (see photo to the right), a pattern that may reduce the chance of prey escaping by maximizing the area of the foot & may allow owls to better subdue larger prey than similar-sized hawks. The morphology of owls (shorter & wider tarsometatarsus; see photo below right) also appears to be associated with a stronger grip, while the hindlimbs of hawks & eagles (relatively long and gracile) seem adapted for high-velocity movements. 
     The force produced by talons may be related to time of activity. Owls hunt when light levels are low so if an attacking owl misses its prey, relocating it may be difficult. Hawks are diurnal hunters and can use visual cues during and after an attack. If unable to subdue prey initially, they can relocate prey visually and catch it.  Given the morphological differences and hunting behaviors of these raptors, how well do those characteristics relate to prey-size selection? 
  • Great Horned Owls can take relatively large mammals such as porcupines and skunks, plus large birds like pheasants and quail 
  • Barred Owls prey mainly on medium-sized mammals, including mice and squirrels, as well as amphibians. 
  • Eastern Screech-Owls prey on insects, small birds, and small mammals. 
  • Red-tailed Hawks subsist primarily on rodents and larger mammals such as skunks and rabbits.
  • Red-shouldered Hawks, like Barred Owls, subsist mainly on medium-sized mammals such as squirrels and chipmunks, but also prey on frogs and salamanders.
  • American Kestrels, like Eastern Screech-Owls, eat mostly insects and small mammals. 

In sum, differences in grip force & the hunting behavior of owls and hawks suggest at least a  partial basis for resource partitioning in the eastern  deciduous forests of North America. Each raptor has a unique force production, along with a different time of activity, that would allow for a degree of prey specialization. 


  Photo of the foot of a Great Horned Owl
Great Horned Owl foot 

Photo of the tarsometatarsi of a Red-tailed Hawk and a Great Horned Owl
Tarsometatarsi of a similarly-sized hawk & owl. 
(A) Red-tailed Hawk. (B) Great Horned Owl. 

 


 

Photo of a Short-eared Owl in flight

Photo of a primary feather of a Barred Owl
Barred Owl primary - leading edge below and trailing edge above

Drawing of the leading edge of an owl flight feather
The serrated leading-edge feather of an owl
(Norberg 2002) .
Drawing of an airplace wing with vortex generators
Vortex generators on an airplane wing.

The silent flight of owls -- Noise is generated by vortices produced when air flows over a bird's wing and larger vortices produce more noise. Wings with small saw-toothed projections (vortex generators), like those on the leading edge of owl wings, generate many small vortices instead of large vortices and produce less aerodynamic noise. In addition, the fringe feathers at the trailing edge of the wing (with fewer hooklets at the ends of the barbs) help to break up the sound waves that are generated as air flows over the top of their wings and forms downstream wakes, and the soft down feathers located elsewhere on the wings and legs of owls absorb the remaining sound frequencies above 2,000 hertz and make owls completely silent to their prey. As a bonus, with high angles of attack and at slow speeds, vortex generators stick out of the stagnant air near the surface of the wing, and into the freely moving air outside the boundary layer. This surface layer is typically quite thin, but dramatically reduces speed of the airflow towards the rear of the wing. The vortex generators mix the free stream with the stagnant air to get it moving again, providing considerably more airflow at the rear of the wing and helping to prevent stalling. This process is referred to as 're-energizing the boundary layer.'

 

 


 

 


 
Eurasian Sparrowhawk

 

Unpredictable predators -- The use of space by predators in relation to their prey is a poorly understood aspect of predator-prey interactions. Classic theory suggests that predators should focus their efforts on areas of abundant prey, that is, prey hotspots, whereas game-theoretical models of predator and prey movement suggest that the distribution of predators should match that of their prey's resources. If, however, prey are spatially anchored to one location and these prey have particularly strong antipredator responses that make them difficult to capture with frequent attacks, then predators may be forced to adopt alternative movement strategies to hunt behaviorally responsive prey. Roth and Lima (2007) examined the movement patterns of bird-eating Sharp-shinned Hawks (Accipiter striatus) in an attempt to shed light on hotspot use by predators. Their results suggest that these hawks do not focus on prey hotspots such as bird feeders but instead maintain much spatial and temporal unpredictability in their movements. Hawks seldom revisited the same area, and the few frequently used areas were revisited in a manner consistent with unpredictable returns, giving prey little additional information about risk.

But why wouldn't Sharp-shinned Hawks focus their hunting on the areas with the most potential prey (bird feeders)? One possibility is that behaviorally responsive prey diminish the "hotspot" quality of feeders. Although feeder hotspots are sources of abundant prey, the individuals at such feeders generally benefit from group vigilance as a result of these higher densities. As a result, the vulnerability of the prey may actually be lower at feeders than at other locations. In addition, unpredictable movement may reflect a sort of "prey management" by predators, whereby predators spread their hunting activity over multiple areas in an effort to avoid inflating the antipredator behavior of their prey. This hunting strategy may be effective when prey are anchored to high-resource areas such as feeders and use antipredator behaviors, such as high vigilance, that reduce a predator's attack success if it attacks frequently and predictably.


Bristles are stiff and hairlike, consisting of a central rachis  without vanes, and provide both protective and sensory functions. Bristles occur most prominently around the eyes ("eyelashes"), the lores, the nostrils, and around the rictus (corners) of the mouth. Not all birds have bristles. Rictal bristles are prominent in many insectivorous birds, particularly aerial insectivores like nightjars (Order Caprimulgiformes) and flycatchers (Family Tyrannidae), and may be used as sensory organs to help locate and capture prey, much like mammals use whiskers. In addition, bristles around the mouth may help protect the eyes from food items a bird is trying to capture (Conover and Miller 1980). The photo to the right shows the rictal bristles of a Hooded Warbler.

(Source: www.westol.com/~banding/Pictorial_Highlights_080511_2002.htm)

Photo of the rictal bristles of a Hooded Warbler

 

AOS21-(510104) The form and function of avian rictal bristles from American Ornithological Society on Vimeo.



Seabirds are choking on ocean plastic (video)

 ;

 

Keeping the Lead Out - Protecting the California Condor from WNPA on Vimeo.

 


The avian tongue:

Two photos of the tongue of a Great Cormorant

The tongues of cormorants and other fish-eating species are small because these species swallow prey whole and tongues are not needed to manipulate or position food in the oral cavity.

Fig. 1. Dorsal view of the surface of the lower bill of a Great Cormorant (Phalacrocorax carbo). Arrow shows the tongue with sharpened tip. 1, hyoid bones; 2, laryngeal sulcus with ostium; 3, esophagus. Scale bar, 12 mm.

Fig. 2. Lateral view of the cormorant tongue. The tongue and the small anterior and posterior areas of the mucosa of the bill are covered by white keratinized epithelium. Black arrow shows short base of the tongue. White arrow shows the median crest on the dorsal surface of the tongue. A, anterior; B, posterior. Scale bar, 3 mm (Source: Jackowiak et al. 2006).

Drawing of the tongue of a woodpecker
Source: Birder's World

Drawing of the tip of a woodpecker's tongue

Detailed view of the horny tip (left) of the Guadeloupe Woodpecker tongue in vivo position (Villard and Cuisin 2004).


Photos of the skull of a woodpecker and the hyoid apparatus of a woodpecker
(a) Skull of Great Spotted Woodpecker, (b) hyoid apparatus of a Great Spotted Woodpecker, and
(c) hyoid apparatus of a non-woodpecker (Eurasian Hoopoe) (From: Wang et al. 2011).  

 

  


     Goose tongue -- The dorsal surface of the tongue of Middendorff's Bean Goose (Anser fabalis middendorffii) has an anterior region that extends for five-sixths of its length plus a posterior region. Large conical papillae (indicated by arrowhead to the right) are located in a row between the anterior and posterior regions. On both sides of the anterior region, lingual papillae are compactly distributed, and small numbers of large conical papillae are found between the lingual papillae. The dorsal surface of the tongue is covered by numerous fine processes, which help hold food on the tongue's surface. 
     The taste buds of birds may be located in the upper beak epithelium, in the anterior mandible, and the mandibular epithelium posterior to the tongue. Some taste buds are also located ventrolaterally on the anterior tongue. -- From: Iswasaki (2002).

Drawing of a Bean Goose

Photo of the tongue of a Bean Goose and of lateral side of the tongue of a Bean Goose
Surface structure and histology of the dorsal epithelium of the 
tongue of Middendorff's Bean Goose. (a) Macroscopic dorsal view of the tongue. Arrows show lingual hairs on the lateral sides). (b) Scanning electron micrograph of the lateral side of the tongue. Lingual papillae (arrows) are compactly distributed on the tongue, and large conical papillae (arrowhead) are scattered among them. Scale bars = 10 µm (a) & 500 µm (b). (From: Iwasaki 2002).

Two photos showing the tongue of a Spotted Nutcracker from the top and the side

Fig. 1. Dorsal view of the tongue of the Spotted Nutcracker (Nucifraga caryocatactes). Arrows show two elongated processes of the apex. A, apex, B, body, R, root, LP, laryngeal prominence. Scale bar, 3 mm.

Fig. 2. Lateral view of the tongue of the nutcracker. Arrow shows elongated processes, pointed diagonally, B, body, R, root. Scale bar, 3 mm.

Nutcracker tongue -- Jackowiak et al. (2010) found that a characteristic feature of the Spotted Nutcracker's tongue is the unique structure of the anterior part of the tongue, which has two long and highly keratinized processes — a product of the renewable keratinized layer of the epithelium covering the ventral surface of the tongue. These dagger-like processes, which are a modified “lingual nail,” help nutcrackers lever up and shell seeds, which are transported over the short sulcus-shaped body of the tongue. Another unique feature of the nutcracker's tongue is the groove separating the body from the root. Two rows of highly keratinized, mechanical, conical papillae are located at the junction of the body and the root. These papillae are mechanically protective elements for passing food particles in the form of seeds. The nutcracker tongue is an efficient tool resembling a lever that is helpful in shelling seeds.

 

Nutcrackers break or partially break off the outer scales of the husk of the cone with their beak and then strip the remaining parts of the husk with the lower part of the beak, exposing the seeds that adhere to the base of the husk. Using the keratinized processes of the lingual apex, they then lever the seeds out of the claw-like base of the seed wing.


Drawing of the skull and tongue of a hummingbird
Source: http://www.zoobooks.com/newFrontPage/animals/virtualZoo/animals/h/humingbirds/head2.htm

 


 

Photos showing how nectar is trapped by a hummingbird tongue

Hummingbird tongues are fluid traps, not capillary tubes -- Hummingbird tongues pick up a liquid, calorie-dense food that cannot be grasped, a physical challenge that has long inspired the study of nectar-transport mechanics. Existing biophysical models predict optimal hummingbird foraging on the basis of equations that assume that fluid rises through the tongue in the same way as through capillary tubes. Rico-Guevara and Rubega (2011) found that hummingbird tongues do not function like a pair of tiny, static tubes drawing up floral nectar via capillary action. Instead, the tongue tip is a dynamic liquid-trapping device that dynamically traps nectar by rapidly changing their shape during feeding. High-speed video observations showed that an entire tongue transformation cycle occurs in as little as 1/20th of a second. In addition, the tongue–fluid interactions are identical in both living and dead birds, demonstrating that this mechanism is a function of the tongue structure itself, and therefore highly efficient because no energy expenditure by the bird is required to drive the opening and closing of the trap. These results rule out previous conclusions from capillarity-based models of nectar feeding and highlight the necessity of developing a new biophysical model for nectar intake in hummingbirds.

Links:

Hummingbird tongue tips twist to trap nectar

How the hummingbird tongue really works (with videos)

 

While feeding, the hummingbird’s tongue is flicking in and out of its bill fifteen or twenty times every second. When the hummingbird draws its tongue back into its bill at the end of each lick, its bill squeezes the tongue from above and below, squeezing out the nectar from the two tubes at the end. When the bird sticks its tongue out again, the tubes are able to expand. Compressing the tubes stores potential energy in their walls, so when the tubes start to expand again, that energy is released, and it draws nectar inside. Every time the bird presses its tongue to release a new mouthful of nectar, it’s also re-setting the pump for another mouthful.

Energy and nitrogen balance in a hummingbird -- Keeping fit and healthy on a low-fat, fiber-free diet isn't easy, but despite the nutritional disadvantages of life on a liquid lunch, hummingbirds flourish by supplementing their nectar intake with tiny arthropods. But the beneficial snacks come at a high metabolic price; flies don't sit still, so hummingbirds work hard chasing their protein. Just how much nitrogen a hummingbird extracts from the protein in its Photo of a Green-backed Firecrowndiet, or the amount of effort needed to gather it, wasn't clear, so López-Calleja et al. (2003) began tempting the tiny birds with nitrogen laced nectar and found that although their protein requirements were relatively meager, the tiny creatures' metabolic demands were colossal: 43 kJ day-1!
     López-Calleja et al. (2003) trapped almost 40 Green-backed Firecrowns in central Chile, before transporting them to an aviary in Santiago ready to test out their metabolism. Back in the lab, the team prepared nectar solutions with different concentrations of amino acids to see how much protein the birds needed to maintain a stable body weight. By filming the birds as they sipped from feeders, they measured the amount of energy and nitrogen that the birds consumed. To calculate the bird's nitrogen uptake, they also needed to know how much waste nitrogen the birds lost. So, they collected all of the birds' feces, making sure that none dried out, and measured the nitrogen content. Not surprisingly, the birds that were fed small amounts of protein began losing weight quickly, even though they were able to sip as much high-energy nectar as they wanted. However, the birds that were fed 1.82% nitrogen or more, held their weight. López-Calleja et al. (2003) calculated that the tiny aeronauts need at least 10 mg nitrogen per day to maintain a stable body mass, or else they waste away.
     What does that translate to in terms of flies? López-Calleja et al. (2003) provided the birds with 500 fruit flies to snack on while offering them either an unlimited nectar supply, a restricted nectar intake, or no nectar at all. After five days of access to flies and nectar, the birds were fit and healthy, catching around 150 flies a day, sufficient to supply them with 5% nitrogen. The birds that had a reduced nectar supply also maintained a stable weight, although they went into torpor overnight to conserve energy. But the birds fed flies alone began losing weight, no matter how hard they worked to feed themselves. Fernández, one of the co-authors was surprised that `flies are not a complete food source for hummingbirds'. She suspects that although the flies should supply all of the hummingbirds needs, the birds simply have to work too hard to catch flies to rely on them as their soul food source. -- Kathryn Phillips, Journal of Experimental Biology

Flush–pursuit foragers use exaggerated and animated foraging movements to flush potential insect prey that are then pursued and captured in flight. The Myioborus redstarts comprise 12 species of flush–pursuit warblers found in montane forests of the American tropics and subtropics. All members of the genus have contrasting black-and-white tail feathersDrawing of a redstart that are exposed by spreading the tail during foraging. Mumme (2002) examined plumage pattern and tail-spreading behavior to see how they affected flush–pursuit foraging performance of the Slate-throated Redstart (Myioborus miniatus) in Costa Rica. Although flycatching was the most common foraging tactic used by Slate-throated Redstarts, flush–pursuit prey attacks occurred more frequently following hops in the spread-tail foraging posture than hops in more typical warbler-like posture, suggesting that tail-spreading behavior assists in startling and flushing potential insect prey. The hypothesis that the white tail feathers enhance flush–pursuit foraging was tested by means of a plumage-dyeing experiment. After locating nests, Mumme (2002) captured the male and female and assigned one member of each pair to the experimental treatment group; its mate served as a control. For experimental birds, a permanent marker was used to blacken the white tips of the three outer retrices. For sham-darkened controls,the naturally black tips of the three inner retrices were also ‘‘blackened.’’ Experimental birds with darkened tail feathers were significantly less successful in flush–pursuit foraging, showed a significantly lower overall rate of prey attack, and fed their nestlings at a significantly lower rate than did their sham-darkened mates. For experimental birds, only 7.6% of hops in the spread-tail posture were followed by an attack on a prey item, compared to 20.9% of hops for controls. These results indicate that white tail feathers are critically important in startling potential prey. 

 
American Redstart


Close encounters with possible prey . . . counterfeit predators -- You are a 12-gram, insectivorous, tropical rainforest bird, foraging in shady, tangled, dappled, rustling foliage where edible caterpillars and other insects are likely to shelter. You want to live 10–20 years. You are peering under leaves, poking into rolled ones, searching around stems, exploring bark crevices and other insect hiding places. Abruptly an eye appears, 1–5 cm from your bill. The eye or a portion of it is half seen, obstructed, shadowed, partly out of focus, more or less round, multicolored, and perhaps moving. If you pause a millisecond to ask whether that eye belongs to acceptable prey or to a predator, you are likely to be—and it takes only once—someone’s breakfast. Your innate reaction to the eye must be instant flight, that is, a “startle” coupled with distancing. The bird that must learn to avoid what appears to be a predator’s eye is not long for this world. Now, a safe few meters away, are you going to go back to see whether that was food? No.

You, like billions of other individuals and hundreds of other species for millions of years, have just been a player in an act of natural selection favoring mutations that lead to the multitudes of “false eye” color patterns, “eyespot” patterns, or “facsimiles of eyes” and “faces” adorning tropical caterpillars and pupae. Associated body patterns often suggest other head and facial features, which in turn enhance the eye-like nature of the spots. None of these patterns exactly matches the eyes or face of any particular species of predator; but, even when quickly and partially glimpsed, all give the illusion of an eye or face. These false eyes are mimicking the eyes and faces of such predators of insect-eating birds as snakes, lizards, other birds, and small mammals, as perceived at close range by the insectivorous birds in their natural world. These color patterns, and the birds’ reactions to them, are the evolutionary footprints of predator/prey encounters as shallow as today and as deep as the first terrestrial vertebrate eyes - From: Janzen et al. (2010).

12 photos of insects showing false eyes and faces
Representative caterpillar false eyes and faces

Buccal, or oral, cavity:

Photo of a swiftlet on its nest
Source: http://animal.discovery.com/fansites/jeffcorwin/carnival/flyingace/swiftlet.html

 


Left, Salivary glands of an adult female Chimney Swift during the non-breeding season.
Right, Fully developed salivary glands of an adult female Chimney Swift during nest building
(Figure from Johnston 1958).

Esophagus:



Anhinga swallowing a large fish

 


Cedar Waxwings

 
Note the distended throat of this American Kestrel


Source: http://www.vetmed.ucdavis.edu/whc/Necropsy/Avian.html


Photos of the exterior and inside of a lactating pigeon crop
The non-‘lactating’ pigeon crop (A) has a completely different appearance from that of the ‘lactating’ crop (B). The lactating crop is
more than twice the size of the non- ‘lactating’ crop, with a thickened wall and two very obvious lateral lobes. When the ‘lactating’ crop is
opened (C) the pigeon ‘milk’ is seen as a bed of close-packed discrete rice-shaped pellets that are closely associated with the mucosal surface
of the tissue (From: Gillespie et al. 2011).

Crop 'milk' -- Both male and female Rock Pigeons (Columba livia) produce pigeon ‘milk’ that is fed to their young. Pigeons generally lay two eggs one day apart, which hatch 18 days after they are laid. Two days before the first egg hatches, pigeon ‘milk’ begins to be produced in the crop of the parent birds. A similar substance is produced by flamingos and male Emperor Penguins. The normal function of the crop is food storage. During the process of pigeon ‘lactation’, a curdlike substance is regurgitated from the crop and fed to young pigeons. This 'milk’ is 60% protein and the remainder is mostly fat (32-36%) with a small amount of carbohydrate (1-3%), in addition to the mineral (calcium, potassium, sodium, and phosphorus) content. Pigeon 'milk' also contains IgA antibodies and antioxidants (carotenoids).

 


Owls set beetle trap with dung -  Levey et al. (2004) compared what Burrowing Owls ate when there was a typical litter of dung at the entrances to their nest burrows with their diet when the dung was removed. The owls ate 10 times more beetles when thePhoto of a Burrowing Owl dung was present, suggesting the waste did not build up by accident. Burrowing Owls make their nests in small tunnels, and place a variety of debris, including dung, at the entrance.  After finding that Burrowing Owls also had a high concentration of dung beetles in their diet, Levey et al. (2004) proposed that the owls might be using dung as bait to attract the beetles. To test this hypothesis, they cleared all nest entrances at two colonies of owls of debris, then one owl colony had a typical littering of dung applied while the other was left bare. After four days each entrance was again completely cleared and the situation was reversed. Analysis of the owls' waste clearly showed that when dung was present, the owls feasted on ten times more dung beetles. As Levey says, "this experiment demonstrates that tool use makes a difference to a wild animal". Although it may be tempting to conclude the owls are clever enough to devise this trap, Levey explained: "I don't believe these burrowing animals are aware of the link between the dung they bring in and the beetles they catch". Instead, the baiting may simply have evolved, as owls who happened to collect more dung had a better diet and therefore bred more successfully.  -- Peter Wood, BBC News Online 



The avian stomach is divided into 2 parts:

Photomicrograph of a cross-section through a proventriculus
Photomicrograph (50X) of a cross section through the proventriculus showing folds of mucous membrane (P);
deep proventricular glands (GP); capsule (connective tissue) around the glands (arrow head); muscle layer (m); serosa
(connective tissue) with blood vessels (S), and the lumen (L) (From: Catroxo et al. 1997).

 

Photo of the stomach and small intestine of a bird
Source: http://arbl.cvmbs.colostate.edu/hbooks/pathphys/digestion/herbivores/birds.html

 

Photomicrograph of a section through a bird's gizzard
Photomicrograph (210X) of longitudinal section of the gizzard showing folds of mucous membrane lined by simple
prismatic epithelium (P); simple tubular glands (Gs) in the lamina propria constituted by connective tissue (Lp);
secretion of glands (S) that are continuous with the cuticle (or koilin); (C), part of muscle layer (m), interpersed with bundles of
connective tissue (Tc) (From: Catroxo et al. 1997).

Photomicrograph of the koilin of a parrot
Photomicrograph (400X) of the koilin of an Eclectus Parrot (Eclectus roratus).
Note the regular, columnated structure of the koilin layer (K) and its
association with the glandular epithelium (E) of the ventriculus (From: De Voe et al. 2003).

Photomicrographs of the walls of a chick gizzard
(1) Section through inner lining of a chicken gizzard. A, koilin, B, crypts, C, glands that secrete koilin, D, epithelial surface, E, desquamated epithelial cells,
(2) Mucosa of the gizzard. A, koilin, B, secretion in gland lumens and crypts, and (3) Koilin layer. A, secretion column, B, koilin-layer surface,
C, horizontal stripe indicating a 'pause' in secretion of the koilin, D, cellular debris. (From: Eglitis and Knouff 1962).


Graph showing the stomach pH of six different species of birds
Stomach pH (mean and standard deviation) of White-backed Griffon Vultures, Wandering Albatrosses, Magellanic and Gentoo penguins
(Spheniscus magellanicus and Pygoscelis papua, King Penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus), and Great Cormorants (Phalacrocorax carbo).

Vultures of the seas -- Animals are primarily limited by their capacity to acquire food, yet digestive performance also conditions energy acquisition, and ultimately fitness. Optimal foraging theory predicts that organisms feeding on patchy resources should maximize their food loads within each patch, and should digest these loads quickly to minimize travelling costs between food patches. Grémillet et al. (2012) tested the prediction of high digestive performance in Wandering Albatrosses (Diomedea exulans) that can ingest prey of up to 3 kg, and feed on highly dispersed food resources across the southern ocean. GPS-tracking of 40 Wandering Albatrosses from the Crozet archipelago during the incubation phase confirmed foraging movements of between 475–4705 km, giving the birds access to a variety of prey, including fishery wastes. Using miniaturized, autonomous data recorders placed in the stomach of three birds, the first-ever measurements of gastric pH and temperature in procellariformes were obtained. These revealed surprisingly low pH levels (mean = 1.50 ± 0.13), markedly lower than in other seabirds, and comparable to those of vultures that feed on carrion. Such low stomach pH gives Wandering Albatrosses a strategic advantage because it allows a rapid chemical breakdown of ingested food and rapid digestion. This is useful for feeding on patchy, natural prey, but also on fishery wastes, which might be an important additional food resource for Wandering Albatrosses. It is likely that this physiological characteristic evolved as a response to a diet largely composed of squid, and to a patchy distribution of this food resource resulting in large, infrequent meals. The strategy of Wandering Albatrosses is to cover long distances rapidly and at low costs to increase the probability of encountering dispersed prey patches whose distribution is unpredictable.


A Price Worth Paying -- Birds don't need teeth to grind their food; they solve the mashing problem with a powerful gizzard. But not all gizzards are equal. In fact, Red Knots' gizzards grow larger when the birds put on weight preparing for migration. But they also change size throughout the year. What causes such changes in gizzard size?

   van Gils et al. (2003) served knots that had large and small gizzards (as determined by ultrasonography) a selection of hard intact molluscs and soft mollusc meat and filmed the birds as they ate. Knots with large gizzards consumed far more molluscs with shells than the birds with smaller gizzards. van Gils et al. (2003) also offered the birds a shell-heavy diet, but even the birds with the largest gizzards needed to feed for 16 hours a day to sustain their weight! Birds with smaller gizzards simply couldn't feed fast enough. By allowing them to crush more shell per gizzard-full, larger gizzards gave birds the edge. 

   Thus, even though it is energetically costly for the knots to maintain a larger gizzard, when the bird needs to get the most out of its crunchy diet, it's a price worth paying. So, the birds' gizzards enlarge as they fatten for migration. van Gils et al. (2003) also found the knot's gizzards enlarged when the molluscs begin shrivelling (as their winter food supply dwindles). Because the molluscs' shells stay the same size as the molluscs shrink, the amount of shell a bird must process to eat its fill also increases. But with their larger gizzards, the birds can still make the most of even the crunchiest winter diet!  -- Kathryn Phillips, Journal of Experimental Biology

Photo of a Red Knot

Graph showing frequency distribution of gizzard mass of 920 Red Knots
Frequency distribution of gizzard mass of free-living Red Knots (N=920).



Effect of three-week long gizzard mass treatment on gizzard mass at the start of diet choice experiments. Lines connect estimated gizzard mass
from the same individual Red Knots in the two treatment categories. During the small gizzard treatment, birds received a high digestive quality diet of the flesh from open mussels,
and during the large gizzard treatment birds received a low digestive quality diet of gastropod mudsnails, 
Hydrobia ulvae, that required processing large shell volumes in the gizzard (Mathot et al. 2017)..

 

 


Figure showing changes in gizzard mass with different diets
Reversible size changes in the gizzards of adult Japanese Quail (Coturnix japonica) (a) and Red Knots (Calidris canutus) (b).
Quail were given a diet of alternatively low or high non-digestible fiber content (3% vs. 45%). Within 14 days, they showed a doubling
of the size of their gizzards. Red Knots have strong muscular gizzards for feeding on molluscs. With a change in diet from medium-small
mussels (Mytilus edulis) to a diet of soft food pellets, gizzard mass was reduced by almost 50% in about 8 days. A shift back to a mussel diet
induced about a doubling in gizzard mass in just a few days. As the knots were fed progessively smaller mussels (day 22 to day 46) that are easier
to crush, gizzard mass again declined. A switch back to a soft food pellet diet caused a further decline in gizzard mass. Finally, a switch back to a
mussel diet again cause a rapid increase in gizzard mass (From: Piersma and Drent 2003).

Photo of the interior of a canary gizzard
Canary stomach
Source: http://members.madasafish.com/~grahamwhite/download/gizzard.htm



Ostrich (Struthio camelus) stomach. Note how particle size of material in the gizzard (ventriculus) is smaller than in the proventriculus due to the grinding action of the muscular walls plus small pebbles (gastroliths). The capacity to reduce particle size is related to the metabolic demands of a species. Therefore, particle size reduction is often considered the key digestive difference between ecto- and endotherms that allows endotherms to rely on shorter digesta retention times without losing digestive efficiency, and hence facilitate the high level of food intake necessary to meet their increased metabolic requirements. Oes, esophagus; Prov, proventriculus; Gizz, gizzard; SI, small intestine (From: Fritz et al. 2011).

Gizzards vs. teeth -- A gizzard system is advantageous when the organismal design demands that the site of particle size reduction be close to the center of gravity; as such, the use of a gizzard is usually linked to the primary characteristic of herbivorous birds, flight. In contrast, adaptations for chewing intrinsically increase the weight of the head. The use of the gizzard system has the potential advantages that intake rate is not limited by chewing, that no investment in dental tissue is necessary, and that dental wear is not a determinant of senescence as observed in mammals. The absence of age-dependent tooth wear might even be a contributing factor to the slower onset of senescence in birds as compared to mammals. On the other hand, the use of a gizzard requires the intake of suitable grit or stones—an action that represents, in the few studies where this has actually been quantified in birds, a relevant proportion of feeding time (Fritz et al. 2011).


Long-term preservation of stomach contents in incubating King Penguins -- Male King Penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus) are able to store undigested food in their stomach for up to 3 weeks during their incubation fast. Such an adaptation ensures hatchling survival if their mate's return is delayed. Using small electronic recorders, Thouzeau et al. (2004) studied the change in gastric pH, motility and temperature during the first week of food storage. The pH could be maintained at values as high as 6 throughout the incubation fast, a pH unfavorable for avian gastric proteinase activity. Gastric motility was markedly reduced for most of the incubating birds, with lower motility probably associated with a better conservation of stomach content. Stomach temperature was maintained at around 38°C. The fact that stomach temperature of incubating birds did not show a daily rhythmic fluctuation as seen in non-breeding birds could be due to temperature constraints on embryo development. Thus, this study demonstrates substantial adjustments of pH and gastric motility in incubating King Penguins, which may contribute to the inhibition of digestive gastric processes. Mechanisms underlying these adjustments are probably complex, including a combination of neuronal, humoral, and/or hormonal factors.

 

AOS21-(230101) Gastrointestinal processes and pathways that mediate interactions between avian hosts and their microbes from American Ornithological Society on Vimeo.

Intestine:

Small intestine:


Gastrointestinal tracts of a carnivorous hawk, an omnivorous chicken, and 4 herbivorous birds.
Note the larger size of the crops in omnivores and herbivores, and particularly in the Hoatzin. Ceca are small in hawks and
relatively large in grouse. Although ceca are relatively small in Hoatzins, Emus, and Ostriches, an expanded foregut
(Hoatzins), a much longer midgut (Emus), or a much longer colon (Ostriches) compensates for this (From: Stevens and Hume 1998).

Digestive tract of an Ostrich
GI tract of an Ostrich chick
(P = proventriculus; G = gizzard; S1 = small intestine;
Ca = caeca; L1, L2, & L3  =  sections of large intestine)
(Source: http://www.ianr.unl.edu/pubs/animals/nf251.htm)



Glucose transport in birds -- In contrast with regulation of intestinal glucose transport in mammals, amphibians and fish, intestinal glucose transport does not change with dietary carbohydrate in most birds. This is interesting, because the diets of many birds change with seasons, and the levels of carbohydrate in those diets also vary with season. Nevertheless, intestinal glucose transport rates do not vary with dietary carbohydrate levels in American Robins, House Sparrows, and Yellow-rumped Drawing showing transport through the wall of a small intestineWarblers. The absence of dietary modulation of glucose transport in birds may be due to the predominance of passive glucose transport, probably occurring through the paracellular pathway (i.e., between cells rather than through cells via active transport). If transport were largely passive and dependent on transepithelial concentration gradients, then there would not be any need for specific changes in carrier-mediated (active) transport. For example, passive absorption of nutrients such as fat-soluble vitamins is not subject to modulation by diet.

     Over-reliance on the passive pathway provides metabolic advantages and ecological constraints. It does provide birds with an absorptive process that can deal with rapid and large changes in intestinal sugar concentrations. The passive pathway is also energetically inexpensive to maintain and modulate. However, passive absorption through the paracellular pathway is dependent on concentration gradients. In the absence of a transport system that selects which materials to absorb, this non-discriminatory pathway may also increase vulnerability to toxins, and thus constrain foraging behavior and limit the breadth of the dietary niche of the birds. Another problem is that when luminal sugar concentrations are lower than those in plasma, glucose may diffuse back into the lumen. -- Source: Ferraris (2001).



Photomicrograph showing cross-section of the intestine of a tinamou
Cross-section of the intestine (ileum) of a Spotted Tinamou (Nothura maculosa).
Villi are lined with columnar epithelium (EP), including goblet cells (arrows) that secrete mucus. The muscle
layer includes longitudinal fibers (MI) on the perimeter, circular fibers (Mc), and additional longitudinal fibers
at the base of the villi (muscularis muscosae; MM) (From: Chikilian and de Speroni 1996).

Photos of intestinal villi
Intestinal microvilli ('brush border') of a (A) House Sparrow and (B) Savannah Sparrow. Scale bar = 0.5 µm
(From: Casotti 2001).

    Large intestine:

Drawing of the intestine of a bird
Source: http://www.ahsc.arizona.edu/uac/iacuc/poultry/species.shtml

Drawings showing variation in morphology of avian cecas
Avian ceca. (A) Little Cormorant, Phalacrocorax niger, (B) Cattle Egret, Bubulcus ibis, (C) Cotton Teal, Nettapus coromandelianus,
(D) Crested Serpent Eagle, Spilornis cheela, (E) Common Quail, Coturnix coturnix, (F) Indian Ring Dove, Streptopelia decaocto,
(G) Red-wattled Lapwing, Vanellus indicus, (H) Koel, Eudynamys scolopacea, (I) Spotted Owlet, Athene brama, (J) Indian Roller, Coracias benghalensis,
(K) Eastern Skylark, Alauda gulgula, & (L) Grey Wagtail, Motacilla caspica (From: Clench 1999).


Avian geophagy -- In birds, geophagy (the intentional consumption of soil) is known for geese, parrots, cockatoos, pigeons, cracids, passeriforms, hornbills, & cassuaries. Brightsmith and Muñoz-Najar (2004) observed ten species of psittacids, three species of columbids, and two species of cracids consuming soil from banks of a river in Peru.  They found that preferred soils were deficient in particles large enough to aid in the mechanical breakdown of food and help digestion. Percent clay content and cation exchange capacity (CEC), both predicted to correlate with adsorption of toxins, did not differ between used and unused sites as had been found in a similar study. Instead, preferred soils were more saline and had higher concentrations of exchangeable sodium. This suggests that the choice of soils at their study site was based primarily on sodium content.  Experimental evidence has shown that soils are capable of adsorbing biologically relevant quantities of toxins in vitro and that soil consumption by parrots does reduce the absorption of toxins in vivo. Brightsmith and Muñoz-Najar (2004) did not find evidence that parrots choose soils with greater CEC or clay content, the characteristics that correlate with the capacity to adsorb toxins. Instead, they found that birds chose soils with higher concentrations of sodium. These two findings are not mutually exclusive but instead suggest that there may be a set of conditional rules for soil selection. In situations in which sodium concentrations are variable, the birds appear to choose soils that are highest in sodium (this study). In areas in which sodium concentrations are uniformly high, birds may choose the soils that have the largest ability to adsorb dietary toxins.
Photo showing parrots eating clay
www.pbs.org/wnet/nature/realmacaw/photoessay6.html

Close-up photo of a parrot eating soil

www.pbs.org/wnet/nature/realmacaw/photoessay7.html

 
Blue-headed Parrots at clay lick

 



Cloaca:
Drawing of a goose showing the Bursa of Fabricius


Why are bird feces white? Unlike mammals, birds don't urinate. Their kidneys extract nitrogenous wastes from the bloodstream, but instead of excreting it as urea dissolved in urine as we do, they excrete it in the form of uric acid. Uric acid has a very low solubility in water, so it emerges as a white paste. This material, as well as the output of the intestines, emerges from the bird's cloaca.  Photo of many cormorants

 ;


Drawing of a penguin poohing
Position of model penguin during defaecation and physical parameters used to
calculate rectal pressure necessary to expel fecal material over a distance of 40 cm.

Pressures produced when penguins pooh -- Brooding Chinstrap (Pygoscelis antarctica ) and Adélie (P. adeliae) penguins do not leave their stony nest to defecate, but move to the edge of it, stand up, turn their back nest-outward, bend forward, lift their tail, and shoot The expelled material hits the ground an average of 40 cm away from the bird. Meyer-Rochow and Gal (2004) determined that the pressures involved could be approximated if they knew the (1) distance the feces traveled, (2) density and viscosity of the material, and (3) shape, aperture, and height of the anus above ground. They determined these parameters and calculated that penguins generated pressures of around 10 kPa (77 mm Hg) to expel watery material and 60 kPa (450 mm Hg) to expel material of higher viscosity similar to that of olive oil (forces well above those known for humans). How penguins choose the direction of defecation, and how wind direction factors into that decision, remain unknown.

 


Accessory organs:

Photo of the digestive system of a pigeon
Pigeon liver and pancreas (& other organs)
(http://www.wtamu.edu/~rmatlack/pigeon_dissection/body_cavity.jpg)

Micrograph showing an avian pancreas
Avian Pancreas tissue
(Source: http://www.uga.edu/~poultry/faculty/compton/ps3800/ps3800slides.htm)


Literature Cited:

Brightsmith, D. J. and R. A. Muñoz-Najar. 2004. Avian geophagy and soil characteristics in southeastern Peru. Biotropica 36: 534-543.

Casotti, G. 2001. Luminal morphology of the avian lower intestine: evidence supporting the importance of retrograde peristalsis for water conservation. Anatomical Record 263: 289-296.

Catroxo, M. H. B., M. A. I. Lima, and C. E. M. P. D. M. Cappellaro. 1997. Histological aspects of the stomach (proventriculus and gizzard) of the Red-capped Cardinal (Paroaria gularis gularis). Rev. Chil. Anat. 15: 19-27.

Chikilian, M. and N. B. de Speroni. 1996. Comparative study of the digestive system of three species of tinamou. I. Crypturellus tataupa, Nothoprocta cinerascens, and Nothura maculosa (Aves: Tinamidae). Journal of Morphology 228: 77-88.

Clench, M. H. 1999. The avian cecum: update and motility review. Journal of Experimental Zoology 283: 441-447.

Conover, M. R. and D. E. Miller. 1980. Rictal bristle function in Willow Flycatcher. Condor 82: 469-471.

De Voe, R., L. Degernes, and K. Karli. 2003. Dysplastic koilin causing proventricular obstruction in an Eclectus Parrot (Eclectus roratus). Journal of Avian Medicine and Surgery 17: 27–32.

Duke, G.E. 1994. Anatomy and physiology of the digestive system in fowl. Pages 46-49 in Proc. 21st Annual Carolina Poultry Nutrition Conference, Dec. 7-8, Charlotte, N.C.

Eglitis, I. and R. A. Knouff. 1962. An histological and histochemical analysis of the inner lining and glandular epithelium of the chicken gizzard. American Journal of Anatomy 111:49-65.

Einoder, L. and A. Richardson. 2006. An ecomorphological study of the raptorial digital tendon locking mechanism. Ibis 148: 515-525.

Ferraris, R. P. 2001. Dietary and developmental regulation of intestinal sugar transport. Biochem. J. 360:265-276.

Frei, S., S. Ortmann, M. Kreuzer, J.-M. Hatt, and M. Clauss. 2017. Digesta retention patterns in geese (Anser anser) and turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo) and deduced function of avian caeca. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology A 204: 219-227.

Fritz, J., E. Kienzle, J. Hummel, O. Wings, W. J. Streich, and M. Clauss. 2011. Gizzard vs. teeth, it's a tie: food-processing efficiency in herbivorous birds and mammals and implications for dinosaur feeding strategies. Paleobiology 37: 577-586.

Gillespie, M. J., V. R. Haring, K. A. McColl, P. Monaghan, J. A. Donald, K. R. Nicholas, R. J. Moore, and T. M. Crowley. 2011. Histological and global gene expression analysis of the 'lactating' pigeon crop. BMC Genomics 12: 452.

Grémillet, D., A. Prudor, Y. le Maho, and H. Weimerskirch. 2012. Vultures of the seas: hyperacidic stomachs in Wandering Albatrosses as an adaptation to dispersed food resources, including fishery wastes. PLoS ONE 7: e37834.

Iwasaki, S. 2002. Evolution of the structure and function of the vertebrate tongue. Journal of Anatomy 201:1-14.

Jackowiak, H., W. Andrzejewski, and S. Godynicki. 2006. Light and scanning electron microscopic study of the tongue in the cormorant Phalacrocorax carbo (Phalacrocoracidae, Aves). Zoological Science 23: 161-167.

Jackowiak, H., K. Skieresz-Szewczyk, Z. Kwieciński, J. Trzcielińska-Lorych, and S. Godynicki. 2010. Functional morphology of the tongue in the nutcracker (Nucifraga caryocatactes). Zoological Science 27: 589-594.

Janzen, D. H., W. Hallwachs, and J. M. Burns. 2010. A tropical horde of counterfeit predator eyes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107: 11659-11665.

Johnstone, D. W. 1958. Sex and age characters and salivary glands of the Chimney Swift. Condor 60: 73-84.

Karasov, W.H. 1990. Digestion in birds: chemical and physiological determinants and ecological implications. Stud. Avian Biol. 13: 391–415.

Klasing, K.C. 1998. Comparative Avian Nutrition. CAB International, New York, NY.

Knudson, E. I. 2002. Instructed learning in the auditory localization pathway of the Barn Owl. Nature 417:322-328.

Ladyguin, A. 2000. The morphology of the bill apparatus in the Steller's Sea Eagle. Pp. 1 - 10 in First Symposium on Steller's and White-tailed Sea Eagles in east Asia (M. Veta and M.J. McGrady, eds.). Wild Bird Society of Japan, Tokyo.

Levey, D. J., R. S. Duncan, & C. F. Levins. 2004. Animal behaviour: Use of dung as a tool by burrowing owls. Nature 431: 1038-1039.

López-Calleja, M. V., M. J. Fernández, and F. Bozinovic. 2003. The integration of energy and nitrogen balance in the hummingbird Sephanoides sephaniodes. J. Exp. Biol. 206:3349 -3359.

Mathot, K. J., A. Dekinga, and T. Piersma. 2017. An experimental test of state–behaviour feedbacks: gizzard mass and foraging behaviour in Red Knots. Functional Ecology 31: 1111-1121.

McWhorter, T. J. and C. Martinez del Rio. 2000. Does gut function limit hummingbird food intake? Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 73:313-324.

Meyer-Rochow, V. B. and J. Gal. 2004. Pressures produced when penguins pooh—calculations on avian defaecation. Polar Biology 27: 56-58.

Mumme, R. L. 2002. Scare tactics in a neotropical warbler: white tail feathers enhance flush-pursuit foraging performance in the Slate-throated Redstart (Myioborus miniatus). Auk 119:1024-1035.

Navalon, G., J. A. Bright, J. Marugan-Lobon, and E. J. Rayfield. 2018. The evolutionary relationship among beak shape, mechanical advantage, and feeding ecology in modern birds. Evolution 73: 422-435.

Pennycuick, C. J. 2008. Gliding flight and soaring. In: Theoretical Ecology Series, vol. 5. Modelling the flying bird (C. J. Pennycuick, ed.), pp. 271-304. Elsevier Inc., Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

Norberg, U. M. L. 2002. Structure, form, and function of flight in engineering and the living world. Journal of Morphology 252: 52-81.

Pauw, A. 1998. Pollen transfer on birds' tongues. Nature 394:731-732.

Piersma, T. and J. Drent. 2003. Phenotypic flexibility and the evolution of organismal design. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 18: 228-233.

Pigot, A. L., C. Sheard, E. T. Miller, T. P. Bregman, B. G. Freeman, U. Roll, N. Seddon, C. H. Trisos, B. C. Weeks, and J. A. Tobias. 2020. Macroevolutionary convergence connects morphological form to ecological function in birds. Nature Ecology & Evolution 4: 230-239.

Prakash, M., D. Quéré, and J. W. M. Bush. 2008. Surface tension transport of prey by feeding shorebirds: the capillary ratchet. Science 320: 931-934.

Rico-Guevara, A., and M. A. Rubega. 2011. The hummingbird tongue is a fluid trap, not a capillary tube. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 108: 9356-9360.

Ropert-Coudert, Y., D. Grémillet, P. Ryan, A. Kato, Y. Naito, and Y. Le Maho. 2004. Between air and water: the plunge dive of the Cape Gannet Morus capensis.
Ibis 146: 281-290.

Ross, E. 2014. Woodpeckers and the biomechanics of concussion. M. S. thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.

Roth, T. C., II, and S. L. Lima. 2007. Use of prey hotspots by an avian predator: purposeful unpredictability? American Naturalist 169: 264-273.

Seki, Y., M. S. Schneider, and M. A. Myers. 2005. Structure and mechanical behavior of a toucan beak. Acta Materialia 53: 5281-5296.

Sereno, P. C., Z. Xijin, and T. Lin. 2010. A new psittacosaur from Inner Mongolia and the parrot-like structure and function of the psittacosaur skull. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 277: 199-209.

Shifferman, E. and D. Eilam. 2004. Movement and direction of movement of a simulated prey affect the success rate in Barn Owl Tyto alba attack. Journal of Avian Biology 35: 111-116.

Stevens, C. E. and I. D. Hume. 1998. Contributions of Microbes in Vertebrate Gastrointestinal Tract to Production and Conservation of Nutrients. Physiological Reviews 78: 393-427.

Sustaita, D. 2008. Musculoskeletal underpinnings to differences in killing behavior between North American accipiters (Falconiformes: Accipitridae) and falcons (Falconidae). Journal of Morphology 269: 283-301.

Thouzeau, C., G. Peters, C. Le Bohec, and Y. Le Maho1. 2004. Adjustments of gastric pH, motility and temperature during long-term preservation of stomach contents in free-ranging incubating King Penguins. Journal of Experimental Biology 207: 2715-2724.

van der Meij, M. A. A. 2005. A tough nut to crack. Adaptations to seed cracking in finches. Ph. D. diss., Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands.

van Gils, J. A., T. Piersma, A. Dekinga, and M. W. Dietz. 2003. Cost-benefit analysis of mollusc-eating in a shorebird. II. Optimizing gizzard size in the face of seasonal demands. J. Exp. Biol. 206:3369 -3380.

Villard, P. and J. Cuisin. 2004. How do woodpeckers extract grubs with their tongues? A study of the Guadeloupe Woodpecker in the French West Indies. Auk 121:509-514.

Wang, L., J. T.-M. Cheung, F. Pu, D. Li, M. Zhang, and Y. Fan. 2011. Why do woodpeckers resist head impact injury: a biomechanical investigation. PLoS ONE 6: e26490.

Ward, A. B., P. D. Weigl, and R. M. Conroy. 2002. Functional morphology of raptor hindlimbs: implications for resource partitioning. Auk 119: 1052-1063.

Warrick, D. R. 198. The turning- and linear-maneuvering performance of birds: the costs of efficiency for coursing insectivores. Canadian Journal of Zoology 76: 1063-1079.

Yanega, G. M., and M. A. Rubega. 2004. Hummingbird jaw bends to aid insect capture. Nature 428: 615.

Yoon, S.-H., and S. Park. 2011. A mechanical analysis of woodpecker drumming and its application to shock-absorbing systems. Bioinspiration & Biomemetics 6: 1-12.


More Lecture Notes:

    I - Introduction to Birds

    II - Bird Flight  I

   III - Bird Flight II

    IV - The Geography of Birds I

    V - The Geography of Birds II

    VII - Circulatory System


Back to Avian Biology

Back to BIO 554/754 syllabus



Photo of a toucan

Photo micrograph of the outer surface of a toucan's bill
The beak's outer shell is made of hexagonal keratin tiles cemented together with an organic glue and piled in several staggered layers.

Drawing showing the exterior and interior of a toucan's bill
Micrograph of the interior of a toucan's bill
The interior of the beak is rigid "foam" composed of bony fibers and drum-like membranes sandwiched between outer layers of keratin. The "foam" is covered with overlapping keratin tiles, each about 50 µm in diameter and 1 µm thick, glued together to form sheets.The closed, air-filled spaces reduce overall weight without loss of rigidity.

Toucan bills: strong but light -- Toucans live in the jungle canopies of South America and feed on tree fruits growing at the ends of branches. They perch on sturdier portions of a branch and use their long beak to reach their food. So, the beak must be rigid enough to resist bending and twisting forces, but has to be light or the bird couldn't get off the ground. Incredibly, a toucan's beak that comprises about 1/3 of its body length, only makes up about 1/20 of a toucan's mass. The lightweight strength of the toucan's beak is due to a matrix of bony fibers and drum-like membranes sandwiched between an outer layer of keratin. Seki et al. (2005) found that the secret to the toucan beak's lightweight strength is an unusual bio-composite. The interior of the beak is rigid "foam" composed of bony fibers and drum-like membranes sandwiched between outer layers of keratin.

 
Toucans

The relation between rate of success and direction of movement for a food item that was pulled forward (a), backward (b) and sideways (c). Direction of prey progression – dotted arrow (1), direction of owl flight – dashed arrow (2), and direction to which the owl had to move its head or trunk – solid arrow (3). Owl picture from Knudsen (2002).

Movement and direction of prey affect raptor success rate -- Shifferman and Eilam (2004) tested a novel idea, that rather than maximizing their distance from a predator during close-distance encounters, prey species are better off moving directly or diagonally toward the predator in order to increase the relative speed and confine the attack to a single available clashing point. They used two tamed Barn Owls (Tyto alba) to measure the rate of attack success in relation to the direction of prey movement. A dead mouse or chick was used to simulate the prey, pulled to various directions by means of a transparent string during the owl's attack. Both owls showed a high success rate in catching stationary compared with moving food items (90% and 21%, respectively). Success was higher when the prey moved directly away, rather than towards the owls (50% and 18%, respectively). Strikingly, these owls had 0% success in catching food items that were pulled sideways. This failure to catch prey that move sideways may reflect constraints in postural head movements in aerial raptors that cannot move the eyes, but rather move the entire head in tracking prey. So far there is no evidence that defensive behavior in terrestrial prey species takes advantage of the above escape directions to lower rates of predator success. However, birds seem to adjust their defensive tactics in the vertical domain by taking-off at a steep angle, thus moving diagonally toward the direction of an approaching aerial predator. These preliminary findings warrant further studies in Barn Owls and other predators, in both field and laboratory settings, to uncover fine predator head movements during hunting, the corresponding defensive behavior of the prey, and the adaptive significance of these behaviors.